IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/plo/pone00/0177261.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Statistical learning in social action contexts

Author

Listed:
  • Claire Monroy
  • Marlene Meyer
  • Sarah Gerson
  • Sabine Hunnius

Abstract

Sensitivity to the regularities and structure contained within sequential, goal-directed actions is an important building block for generating expectations about the actions we observe. Until now, research on statistical learning for actions has solely focused on individual action sequences, but many actions in daily life involve multiple actors in various interaction contexts. The current study is the first to investigate the role of statistical learning in tracking regularities between actions performed by different actors, and whether the social context characterizing their interaction influences learning. That is, are observers more likely to track regularities across actors if they are perceived as acting jointly as opposed to in parallel? We tested adults and toddlers to explore whether social context guides statistical learning and—if so—whether it does so from early in development. In a between-subjects eye-tracking experiment, participants were primed with a social context cue between two actors who either shared a goal of playing together (‘Joint’ condition) or stated the intention to act alone (‘Parallel’ condition). In subsequent videos, the actors performed sequential actions in which, for certain action pairs, the first actor’s action reliably predicted the second actor’s action. We analyzed predictive eye movements to upcoming actions as a measure of learning, and found that both adults and toddlers learned the statistical regularities across actors when their actions caused an effect. Further, adults with high statistical learning performance were sensitive to social context: those who observed actors with a shared goal were more likely to correctly predict upcoming actions. In contrast, there was no effect of social context in the toddler group, regardless of learning performance. These findings shed light on how adults and toddlers perceive statistical regularities across actors depending on the nature of the observed social situation and the resulting effects.

Suggested Citation

  • Claire Monroy & Marlene Meyer & Sarah Gerson & Sabine Hunnius, 2017. "Statistical learning in social action contexts," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 12(5), pages 1-20, May.
  • Handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0177261
    DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177261
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0177261
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0177261&type=printable
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1371/journal.pone.0177261?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. J. Randall Flanagan & Roland S. Johansson, 2003. "Action plans used in action observation," Nature, Nature, vol. 424(6950), pages 769-771, August.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Francesca Foti & Deny Menghini & Laura Mandolesi & Francesca Federico & Stefano Vicari & Laura Petrosini, 2013. "Learning by Observation: Insights from Williams Syndrome," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 8(1), pages 1-10, January.
    2. Jairo Perez-Osorio & Hermann J Müller & Eva Wiese & Agnieszka Wykowska, 2015. "Gaze Following Is Modulated by Expectations Regarding Others’ Action Goals," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(11), pages 1-19, November.
    3. Ariel Goldstein & Ido Rivlin & Alon Goldstein & Yoni Pertzov & Ran R Hassin, 2020. "Predictions from masked motion with and without obstacles," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 15(11), pages 1-35, November.
    4. Anne Keitel & Wolfgang Prinz & Moritz M Daum, 2014. "Perception of Individual and Joint Action in Infants and Adults," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 9(9), pages 1-9, September.
    5. Christian Seegelke & Charmayne Mary Lee Hughes & Thomas Schack, 2013. "Simulating My Own or Others Action Plans? – Motor Representations, Not Visual Representations Are Recalled in Motor Memory," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 8(12), pages 1-1, December.
    6. Ettore Ambrosini & Vasudevi Reddy & Annette de Looper & Marcello Costantini & Beatriz Lopez & C Sinigaglia, 2013. "Looking Ahead: Anticipatory Gaze and Motor Ability in Infancy," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 8(7), pages 1-9, July.
    7. Dmitry Smirnov & Fanny Lachat & Tomi Peltola & Juha M Lahnakoski & Olli-Pekka Koistinen & Enrico Glerean & Aki Vehtari & Riitta Hari & Mikko Sams & Lauri Nummenmaa, 2017. "Brain-to-brain hyperclassification reveals action-specific motor mapping of observed actions in humans," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 12(12), pages 1-23, December.
    8. Maurits Adam & Birgit Elsner, 2020. "The impact of salient action effects on 6-, 7-, and 11-month-olds’ goal-predictive gaze shifts for a human grasping action," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 15(10), pages 1-18, October.
    9. Miya K Rand & Sebastian Rentsch, 2016. "Eye-Hand Coordination during Visuomotor Adaptation with Different Rotation Angles: Effects of Terminal Visual Feedback," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 11(11), pages 1-31, November.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0177261. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: plosone (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/ .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.