IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/beheco/v24y2013i2p412-420..html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Vocal deviation and trill consistency do not affect male response to playback in house wrens

Author

Listed:
  • Emily R.A. Cramer

Abstract

Signals that require a high degree of skill to produce are expected to honestly indicate signaler quality. In trilled birdsong, 2 parameters that likely reflect performance difficulty are vocal deviation (how rapidly sound frequency is modulated) and trill consistency (how precisely syllables are repeated). These parameters function as intra- and intersexual signals in most bird species tested to date, but they may not adequately capture song performance difficulty in all species. I used 2 playback protocols to test whether males respond differently to songs that differ in vocal deviation and trill consistency in house wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Despite large sample sizes, male responses did not depend on playback treatment. Males sang each trill type at a range of pitches, and the vocal deviation of the trill depended strongly on the pitch at which it was sung, consistent with models of song production mechanics. I propose that the addition of the pitch covariate may complicate the evaluation of vocal deviation, limiting the usefulness of this potential signal for this species. Moreover, producing each trill type at a range of pitches may itself serve some communication function, which could override the potential signal value of trill consistency. Although vocal deviation and trill consistency are male quality indicators in some species, these results suggest that species-specific differences in what constitutes a "challenging" song may prevent these measures from being universally applicable.

Suggested Citation

  • Emily R.A. Cramer, 2013. "Vocal deviation and trill consistency do not affect male response to playback in house wrens," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(2), pages 412-420.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:24:y:2013:i:2:p:412-420.
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/ars178
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. Bruce E. Byers, 2007. "Extrapair paternity in chestnut-sided warblers is correlated with consistent vocal performance," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 18(1), pages 130-136, January.
    2. Samuel P. Caro & Kendra B. Sewall & Katrina G. Salvante & Keith W. Sockman, 2010. "Female Lincoln's sparrows modulate their behavior in response to variation in male song quality," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 21(3), pages 562-569.
    3. Gonçalo C. Cardoso & Jonathan W. Atwell & Ellen D. Ketterson & Trevor D. Price, 2009. "Song types, song performance, and the use of repertoires in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis)," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 20(4), pages 901-907.
    4. Gonçalo C. Cardoso & Jonathan W. Atwell & Ellen D. Ketterson & Trevor D. Price, 2007. "Inferring performance in the songs of dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis)," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 18(6), pages 1051-1057.
    5. Katie LaBarbera & Paulo E. Llambías & Emily R. A. Cramer & Taza D. Schaming & Irby J. Lovette, 2010. "Synchrony does not explain extrapair paternity rate variation in northern or southern house wrens," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 21(4), pages 773-780.
    6. D. H. Brunton & B. Evans & T. Cope & W. Ji, 2008. "A test of the dear enemy hypothesis in female New Zealand bellbirds (Anthornis melanura): female neighbors as threats," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 19(4), pages 791-798.
    7. Barbara Ballentine & Jeremy Hyman & Stephen Nowicki, 2004. "Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 15(1), pages 163-168, January.
    8. Selvino R. de Kort & Erin R. B. Eldermire & Emily R. A. Cramer & Sandra L. Vehrencamp, 2009. "The deterrent effect of bird song in territory defense," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 20(1), pages 200-206.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Citations

    Citations are extracted by the CitEc Project, subscribe to its RSS feed for this item.
    as


    Cited by:

    1. Benjamin M. Davidson & Gabriela Antonova & Haven Dlott & Jesse R. Barber & Clinton D. Francis, 2017. "Natural and anthropogenic sounds reduce song performance: insights from two emberizid species," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 28(4), pages 974-982.

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Javier Sierro & Selvino R. Kort & Ian R. Hartley, 2023. "Sexual selection for both diversity and repetition in birdsong," Nature Communications, Nature, vol. 14(1), pages 1-13, December.
    2. Angelika Poesel & Douglas A. Nelson & H. Lisle Gibbs, 2012. "Song sharing correlates with social but not extrapair mating success in the white-crowned sparrow," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(3), pages 627-634.
    3. Vicente García-Navas & Esperanza S. Ferrer & Javier Bueno-Enciso & Rafael Barrientos & Juan José Sanz & Joaquín Ortego, 2014. "Extrapair paternity in Mediterranean blue tits: socioecological factors and the opportunity for sexual selection," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(1), pages 228-238.
    4. Grames, Eliza M. & Stepule, Piper L. & Herrick, Susan Z. & Ranelli, Benjamin T. & Elphick, Chris S., 2022. "Separating acoustic signal into underlying behaviors with self-exciting point process models," Ecological Modelling, Elsevier, vol. 468(C).
    5. Mary J. Montague & Marine Danek-Gontard & Hansjoerg P. Kunc, 2013. "Phenotypic plasticity affects the response of a sexually selected trait to anthropogenic noise," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(2), pages 343-348.
    6. Sandra L. Vehrencamp & Jesse M. Ellis & Brett F. Cropp & John M. Koltz, 2014. "Negotiation of territorial boundaries in a songbird," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(6), pages 1436-1450.
    7. Jason Keagy & Jean-François Savard & Gerald Borgia, 2012. "Cognitive ability and the evolution of multiple behavioral display traits," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(2), pages 448-456.
    8. Gabe Winter & Luis Wirsching & Holger Schielzeth, 2023. "Condition dependence of (un)predictability in escape behavior of a grasshopper species," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 34(5), pages 741-750.
    9. Samuel P Caro & Léo Pierre & Matthieu Bergès & Raldi Bakker & Claire Doutrelant & Francesco Bonadonna, 2021. "Mutual mate preferences and assortative mating in relation to a carotenoid-based color trait in blue tits," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 32(6), pages 1171-1182.
    10. Cory A. Toth & Daniel J. Mennill & Laurene M. Ratcliffe, 2012. "Evidence for multicontest eavesdropping in chickadees," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(4), pages 836-842.
    11. Puya Abbassi & Nancy Tyler Burley, 2012. "Nice guys finish last: same-sex sexual behavior and pairing success in male budgerigars," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(4), pages 775-782.
    12. Ada M. Grabowska-Zhang & Teddy A. Wilkin & Ben C. Sheldon, 2012. "Effects of neighbor familiarity on reproductive success in the great tit (Parus major)," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(2), pages 322-333.
    13. Michael S. Reichert, 2014. "Playback tests and studies of animal contest dynamics: concepts and an example in the gray tree frog," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(3), pages 591-603.
    14. Gonçalo C. Cardoso, 2012. "Paradoxical calls: the opposite signaling role of sound frequency across bird species," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(2), pages 237-241.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:24:y:2013:i:2:p:412-420.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.