IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/nat/nathum/v9y2025i2d10.1038_s41562-024-01961-1.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Predicting the replicability of social and behavioural science claims in COVID-19 preprints

Author

Listed:
  • Alexandru Marcoci

    (University of Cambridge
    University of Nottingham)

  • David P. Wilkinson

    (University of Melbourne
    University of Melbourne)

  • Ans Vercammen

    (University of Melbourne
    The University of Queensland
    Curtin University)

  • Bonnie C. Wintle

    (University of Melbourne)

  • Anna Lou Abatayo

    (Wageningen University and Research)

  • Ernest Baskin

    (Saint Joseph’s University)

  • Henk Berkman

    (University of Auckland)

  • Erin M. Buchanan

    (Harrisburg University of Science and Technology)

  • Sara Capitán

    (Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences)

  • Tabaré Capitán

    (Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences)

  • Ginny Chan

    (Rhizom Psychological Services LLC)

  • Kent Jason G. Cheng

    (The Pennsylvania State University)

  • Tom Coupé

    (University of Canterbury)

  • Sarah Dryhurst

    (University of Cambridge
    University of Cambridge
    University College London)

  • Jianhua Duan

    (Statistics New Zealand)

  • John E. Edlund

    (Rochester Institute of Technology)

  • Timothy M. Errington

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Anna Fedor

    (Independent researcher)

  • Fiona Fidler

    (University of Melbourne)

  • James G. Field

    (West Virginia University)

  • Nicholas Fox

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Hannah Fraser

    (University of Melbourne)

  • Alexandra L. J. Freeman

    (University of Cambridge)

  • Anca Hanea

    (University of Melbourne
    University of Melbourne)

  • Felix Holzmeister

    (University of Innsbruck)

  • Sanghyun Hong

    (University of Canterbury)

  • Raquel Huggins

    (Harrisburg University of Science and Technology)

  • Nick Huntington-Klein

    (Seattle University)

  • Magnus Johannesson

    (Stockholm School of Economics)

  • Angela M. Jones

    (Texas State University)

  • Hansika Kapoor

    (Monk Prayogshala
    University of Connecticut)

  • John Kerr

    (University of Cambridge
    University of Otago)

  • Melissa Kline Struhl

    (Massachusetts Institute of Technology)

  • Marta Kołczyńska

    (Polish Academy of Sciences)

  • Yang Liu

    (University of California, Santa Cruz)

  • Zachary Loomas

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Brianna Luis

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Esteban Méndez

    (Central Bank of Costa Rica)

  • Olivia Miske

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Fallon Mody

    (University of Melbourne
    University of Melbourne)

  • Carolin Nast

    (University of Stavanger, School of Business and Law)

  • Brian A. Nosek

    (Center for Open Science
    University of Virginia)

  • E. Simon Parsons

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Thomas Pfeiffer

    (Massey University)

  • W. Robert Reed

    (University of Canterbury)

  • Jon Roozenbeek

    (University of Cambridge)

  • Alexa R. Schlyfestone

    (Harrisburg University of Science and Technology)

  • Claudia R. Schneider

    (University of Cambridge
    University of Cambridge
    University of Canterbury)

  • Andrew Soh

    (University of Hawaii at Manoa)

  • Zhongchen Song

    (New Zealand Institute of Economic Research (NZIER))

  • Anirudh Tagat

    (Monk Prayogshala)

  • Melba Tutor

    (Independent researcher)

  • Andrew H. Tyner

    (Center for Open Science)

  • Karolina Urbanska

    (Independent researcher)

  • Sander Linden

    (University of Cambridge)

Abstract

Replications are important for assessing the reliability of published findings. However, they are costly, and it is infeasible to replicate everything. Accurate, fast, lower-cost alternatives such as eliciting predictions could accelerate assessment for rapid policy implementation in a crisis and help guide a more efficient allocation of scarce replication resources. We elicited judgements from participants on 100 claims from preprints about an emerging area of research (COVID-19 pandemic) using an interactive structured elicitation protocol, and we conducted 29 new high-powered replications. After interacting with their peers, participant groups with lower task expertise (‘beginners’) updated their estimates and confidence in their judgements significantly more than groups with greater task expertise (‘experienced’). For experienced individuals, the average accuracy was 0.57 (95% CI: [0.53, 0.61]) after interaction, and they correctly classified 61% of claims; beginners’ average accuracy was 0.58 (95% CI: [0.54, 0.62]), correctly classifying 69% of claims. The difference in accuracy between groups was not statistically significant and their judgements on the full set of claims were correlated (r(98) = 0.48, P

Suggested Citation

  • Alexandru Marcoci & David P. Wilkinson & Ans Vercammen & Bonnie C. Wintle & Anna Lou Abatayo & Ernest Baskin & Henk Berkman & Erin M. Buchanan & Sara Capitán & Tabaré Capitán & Ginny Chan & Kent Jason, 2025. "Predicting the replicability of social and behavioural science claims in COVID-19 preprints," Nature Human Behaviour, Nature, vol. 9(2), pages 287-304, February.
  • Handle: RePEc:nat:nathum:v:9:y:2025:i:2:d:10.1038_s41562-024-01961-1
    DOI: 10.1038/s41562-024-01961-1
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-024-01961-1
    File Function: Abstract
    Download Restriction: Access to the full text of the articles in this series is restricted.

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1038/s41562-024-01961-1?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nathum:v:9:y:2025:i:2:d:10.1038_s41562-024-01961-1. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.