IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/gam/jijerp/v18y2021i4p1370-d492113.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Anhedonia as a Potential Risk Factor of Alzheimer’s Disease in a Community-Dwelling Elderly Sample: Results from the ZARADEMP Project

Author

Listed:
  • David Vaquero-Puyuelo

    (Psychiatry Service, Hospital Clínico Universitario Lozano Blesa, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Department of Medicine, Psychiatry and Dermatology, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Both authors contributed equally.)

  • Concepción De-la-Cámara

    (Psychiatry Service, Hospital Clínico Universitario Lozano Blesa, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Department of Medicine, Psychiatry and Dermatology, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Aragón (IIS Aragón), 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain)

  • Beatriz Olaya

    (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain
    Research, Innovation and Teaching Unit, Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Universitat de Barcelona, 08007 Barcelona, Spain)

  • Patricia Gracia-García

    (Department of Medicine, Psychiatry and Dermatology, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Aragón (IIS Aragón), 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain
    Psychiatry Service, Hospital Universitario Miguel Servet, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain)

  • Antonio Lobo

    (Department of Medicine, Psychiatry and Dermatology, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Aragón (IIS Aragón), 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain)

  • Raúl López-Antón

    (Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Aragón (IIS Aragón), 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain
    Department of Psychology and Sociology, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain)

  • Javier Santabárbara

    (Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Aragón (IIS Aragón), 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
    Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Ministry of Science and Innovation, 28029 Madrid, Spain
    Department of Microbiology, Pediatrics, Radiology and Public Health, Universidad de Zaragoza, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain)

Abstract

(1) Introduction: Dementia is a major public health problem, and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most frequent subtype. Clarifying the potential risk factors is necessary in order to improve dementia-prevention strategies and quality of life. Here, our purpose was to investigate the role of the absence of hedonic tone; anhedonia, understood as the reduction on previous enjoyable daily activities, which occasionally is underdetected and underdiagnosed; and the risk of developing AD in a cognitively unimpaired and non-depressed population sample. (2) Method: We used data from the Zaragoza Dementia and Depression (ZARADEMP) project, a longitudinal epidemiological study on dementia and depression. After excluding subjects with dementia, a sample of 2830 dwellers aged ≥65 years was followed for 4.5 years. The geriatric mental state examination was used to identify cases of anhedonia. AD was diagnosed by a panel of research psychiatrists according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria. A multivariate survival analysis and Cox proportional hazards regression model were performed, and the analysis was controlled by an analysis for the presence of clinically significant depression. (3) Results: We found a significant association between anhedonia cases and AD risk in the univariate analysis (hazard ratio (HR): 2.37; 95% CI: 1.04–5.40). This association persisted more strongly in the fully adjusted model. (4) Conclusions: Identifying cognitively intact individuals with anhedonia is a priority to implement preventive strategies that could delay the progression of cognitive and functional impairment in subjects at risk of AD.

Suggested Citation

  • David Vaquero-Puyuelo & Concepción De-la-Cámara & Beatriz Olaya & Patricia Gracia-García & Antonio Lobo & Raúl López-Antón & Javier Santabárbara, 2021. "Anhedonia as a Potential Risk Factor of Alzheimer’s Disease in a Community-Dwelling Elderly Sample: Results from the ZARADEMP Project," IJERPH, MDPI, vol. 18(4), pages 1-12, February.
  • Handle: RePEc:gam:jijerp:v:18:y:2021:i:4:p:1370-:d:492113
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/18/4/1370/pdf
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/18/4/1370/
    Download Restriction: no
    ---><---

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. Mathias Pessiglione & Ben Seymour & Guillaume Flandin & Raymond J. Dolan & Chris D. Frith, 2006. "Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans," Nature, Nature, vol. 442(7106), pages 1042-1045, August.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Antoine Collomb-Clerc & Maëlle C. M. Gueguen & Lorella Minotti & Philippe Kahane & Vincent Navarro & Fabrice Bartolomei & Romain Carron & Jean Regis & Stephan Chabardès & Stefano Palminteri & Julien B, 2023. "Human thalamic low-frequency oscillations correlate with expected value and outcomes during reinforcement learning," Nature Communications, Nature, vol. 14(1), pages 1-10, December.
    2. Manuel Glauco Carbone & Icro Maremmani, 2024. "Chronic Cocaine Use and Parkinson’s Disease: An Interpretative Model," IJERPH, MDPI, vol. 21(8), pages 1-23, August.
    3. Hanan Shteingart & Tal Neiman & Yonatan Loewenstein, 2012. "The Role of First Impression in Operant Learning," Discussion Paper Series dp626, The Federmann Center for the Study of Rationality, the Hebrew University, Jerusalem.
    4. Sophie Massin, 2011. "La notion d'addiction en économie : La théorie du choix rationnel à l'épreuve," Revue d'économie politique, Dalloz, vol. 121(5), pages 713-750.
    5. Kuhnen, Camelia M. & Miu, Andrei C., 2017. "Socioeconomic status and learning from financial information," Journal of Financial Economics, Elsevier, vol. 124(2), pages 349-372.
    6. Gifford Jr., Adam, 2009. "Cultural, cognition and human action," Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics (formerly The Journal of Socio-Economics), Elsevier, vol. 38(1), pages 13-24, January.
    7. Hachi E. Manzur & Ksenia Vlasov & You-Jhe Jhong & Hung-Yen Chen & Shih-Chieh Lin, 2023. "The behavioral signature of stepwise learning strategy in male rats and its neural correlate in the basal forebrain," Nature Communications, Nature, vol. 14(1), pages 1-15, December.
    8. Biele, Guido & Rieskamp, Jörg & Krugel, Lea K. & Heekeren, Hauke R., 2011. "The neural basis of following advice," SFB 649 Discussion Papers 2011-038, Humboldt University Berlin, Collaborative Research Center 649: Economic Risk.
    9. Daniel Serra, 2019. "Neuroeconomics and modern neuroscience," CEE-M Working Papers halshs-02160907, CEE-M, Universtiy of Montpellier, CNRS, INRA, Montpellier SupAgro.
    10. He A Xu & Alireza Modirshanechi & Marco P Lehmann & Wulfram Gerstner & Michael H Herzog, 2021. "Novelty is not surprise: Human exploratory and adaptive behavior in sequential decision-making," PLOS Computational Biology, Public Library of Science, vol. 17(6), pages 1-32, June.
    11. Reimann, Martin & Bechara, Antoine, 2010. "The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: Review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research," Journal of Economic Psychology, Elsevier, vol. 31(5), pages 767-776, October.
    12. George I. Christopoulos & Xiao-Xiao Liu & Ying-yi Hong, 2017. "Toward an Understanding of Dynamic Moral Decision Making: Model-Free and Model-Based Learning," Journal of Business Ethics, Springer, vol. 144(4), pages 699-715, September.
    13. Necker, Sarah & Ziegelmeyer, Michael, 2016. "Household risk taking after the financial crisis," The Quarterly Review of Economics and Finance, Elsevier, vol. 59(C), pages 141-160.
    14. Maël Lebreton & Karin Bacily & Stefano Palminteri & Jan B Engelmann, 2019. "Contextual influence on confidence judgments in human reinforcement learning," PLOS Computational Biology, Public Library of Science, vol. 15(4), pages 1-27, April.
    15. Tal Neiman & Yonatan Loewenstein, 2011. "Reinforcement learning in professional basketball players," Discussion Paper Series dp593, The Federmann Center for the Study of Rationality, the Hebrew University, Jerusalem.
    16. Romane Cecchi & Antoine Collomb-Clerc & Inès Rachidi & Lorella Minotti & Philippe Kahane & Mathias Pessiglione & Julien Bastin, 2024. "Direct stimulation of anterior insula and ventromedial prefrontal cortex disrupts economic choices," Nature Communications, Nature, vol. 15(1), pages 1-11, December.
    17. Davide Cenci & Manuel Glauco Carbone & Camilla Callegari & Icro Maremmani, 2022. "Psychomotor Symptoms in Chronic Cocaine Users: An Interpretative Model," IJERPH, MDPI, vol. 19(3), pages 1-10, February.
    18. Daniel Serra, 2021. "Decision-making: from neuroscience to neuroeconomics—an overview," Theory and Decision, Springer, vol. 91(1), pages 1-80, July.
    19. repec:mea:meawpa:14279 is not listed on IDEAS
    20. Kuhnen, Camelia M. & Knutson, Brian, 2011. "The Influence of Affect on Beliefs, Preferences, and Financial Decisions," Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis, Cambridge University Press, vol. 46(3), pages 605-626, June.
    21. Bruno B Averbeck, 2015. "Theory of Choice in Bandit, Information Sampling and Foraging Tasks," PLOS Computational Biology, Public Library of Science, vol. 11(3), pages 1-28, March.

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:gam:jijerp:v:18:y:2021:i:4:p:1370-:d:492113. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: MDPI Indexing Manager (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://www.mdpi.com .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.