IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/pal/palcom/v8y2021i1d10.1057_s41599-021-00943-2.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Intergenerational differences in Russian color naming in the globalized era: linguistic analysis

Author

Listed:
  • Yulia A. Griber

    (Smolensk State University)

  • Dimitris Mylonas

    (New College of the Humanities at Northeastern University)

  • Galina V. Paramei

    (Liverpool Hope University)

Abstract

The present study is an apparent-time analysis of color terms in Russian native speakers (N = 1927), whose age varied between 16 and 98 years. Stratified sampling was employed with the following age groups: 16–19, 20–29, and so on, with the oldest group of 70 years and over. Color names were elicited in a web-based psycholinguistic experiment ( http://colournaming.com ). Participants labeled color samples (N = 606) using an unconstrained color-naming method. Color vocabulary of each age group was estimated using multiple linguistic measures: diversity index; frequency of occurrences of 12 Russian basic color terms (BCTs) and of most frequent non-BCTs; color-naming pattern. Our findings show intergenerational differences in Russian color-term vocabulary, color-naming patterns, and object referents. The CT diversity (measured by the Margalef index) progressively increments with speakers’ juniority; the lexical refinement is manifested by the increasing variety of BCT modifiers and growing use of non-BCTs, both traditional and novel. Furthermore, the most frequent Russian non-BCTs sirenevyj “lilac”, salatovyj “lettuce‐colored”, and birûzovyj “turquoise” appear to be the emerging BCTs. The greatest diversity and richness of CT inventory is observed in Russian speakers aged 20–59 years, i.e., those who constitute the active workforce and are enthusiastic consumers. In comparison, speakers of 60 and over manifest less diverse color inventory and greater prevalence of (modified) BCTs. The two youngest groups (16–29 years) are linguistic innovators: their color vocabulary includes abundant recent loanwords, predominantly from English and, not infrequently, CTs as nouns rather than adjectives. Moreover, Generation Z (16–19 years) tend to offer highly specific or idiosyncratic color descriptors that serve expressive rather than informative function. The apprehended dynamics of color naming in apparent time reflects intergenerational differences as such, but even more so dramatic changes of sociocultural reality in the post-Soviet era, whereby Russian speakers, in particular under 60 years, were/are greatly impacted by globalization of trade: new market product arrivals resulted in adoption of novel and elaboration of traditional CTs for efficient communication about perceived color

Suggested Citation

  • Yulia A. Griber & Dimitris Mylonas & Galina V. Paramei, 2021. "Intergenerational differences in Russian color naming in the globalized era: linguistic analysis," Palgrave Communications, Palgrave Macmillan, vol. 8(1), pages 1-19, December.
  • Handle: RePEc:pal:palcom:v:8:y:2021:i:1:d:10.1057_s41599-021-00943-2
    DOI: 10.1057/s41599-021-00943-2
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://link.springer.com/10.1057/s41599-021-00943-2
    File Function: Abstract
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1057/s41599-021-00943-2?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. Lisa Tabor Connor & Avron Spiro & Loraine K. Obler & Martin L. Albert, 2004. "Change in Object Naming Ability During Adulthood," The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, The Gerontological Society of America, vol. 59(5), pages 203-209.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Citations

    Citations are extracted by the CitEc Project, subscribe to its RSS feed for this item.
    as


    Cited by:

    1. Dimitris Mylonas & Serge Caparos & Jules Davidoff, 2022. "Augmenting a colour lexicon," Palgrave Communications, Palgrave Macmillan, vol. 9(1), pages 1-12, December.
    2. Maria Michela Viva & Serena Castellotti & Galina V. Paramei, 2023. "The Italian colour lexicon in Tuscany: elicited lists, cognitive salience, and semantic maps of colour terms," Palgrave Communications, Palgrave Macmillan, vol. 10(1), pages 1-17, December.

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Hanna S Gauvin & Jolien Mertens & Peter Mariën & Patrick Santens & Barbara A Pickut & Robert J Hartsuiker, 2017. "Verbal monitoring in Parkinson’s disease: A comparison between internal and external monitoring," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 12(8), pages 1-28, August.
    2. Overton, Marieclaire & Pihlsgård, Mats & Elmståhl, Sölve, 2018. "Up to speed: Birth cohort effects observed for speed of processing in older adults: Data from the Good Ageing in Skåne population study," Intelligence, Elsevier, vol. 67(C), pages 33-43.
    3. Gitit Kavé & Shimon Fridkin & Liat Ayalon, 2018. "Demographic factors and retrieval of object and proper names after age 70," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 13(1), pages 1-12, January.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:pal:palcom:v:8:y:2021:i:1:d:10.1057_s41599-021-00943-2. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://www.nature.com/ .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.