IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/beheco/v23y2012i3p481-490..html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Beards augment perceptions of men's age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness

Author

Listed:
  • Barnaby J. Dixson
  • Paul L. Vasey

Abstract

The beard is a strikingly sexually dimorphic androgen-dependent secondary sexual trait in humans. Darwin posited that beards evolved in human ancestors via female choice as a highly attractive masculine adornment. Others have since proposed that beards evolved as a signal of male status and dominance. Here, we show that women from two very different ethnic groups, Europeans from New Zealand and Polynesians from Samoa, do not rate bearded male faces as more attractive than clean-shaven faces. Women and men from both cultures judge bearded faces to be older and ascribe them higher social status than the same men when clean-shaven. Images of bearded men displaying an aggressive facial expression were also rated as significantly more aggressive than the same men when clean-shaven. Thus, the beard appears to augment the effectiveness of human aggressive facial displays. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that the human beard evolved primarily via intrasexual selection between males and as part of complex facial communication signaling status and aggressiveness.

Suggested Citation

  • Barnaby J. Dixson & Paul L. Vasey, 2012. "Beards augment perceptions of men's age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(3), pages 481-490.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:23:y:2012:i:3:p:481-490.
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arr214
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. Markus J. Rantala & Mari Pölkki & Liisa M. Rantala, 2010. "Preference for human male body hair changes across the menstrual cycle and menopause," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 21(2), pages 419-423.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Citations

    Citations are extracted by the CitEc Project, subscribe to its RSS feed for this item.
    as


    Cited by:

    1. Steven C. Hertler, 2015. "Toward a Biology of Collectivism," SAGE Open, , vol. 5(2), pages 21582440155, June.
    2. Borau, Sylvie & Bonnefon, Jean-François, 2020. "Gendered products act as the extended phenotype of human sexual dimorphism: They increase physical attractiveness and desirability," Journal of Business Research, Elsevier, vol. 120(C), pages 498-508.
    3. Ho Fai Chan & Ahmed Skali & David Stadelmann & Benno Torgler & Stephen Whyte, 2021. "Masculinity cues, perceptions of politician attributes, and political behavior," Economics and Politics, Wiley Blackwell, vol. 33(1), pages 148-171, March.
    4. Mittal, Sarah & Silvera, David H., 2021. "It grows on you: Perceptions of sales/service personnel with facial hair," Journal of Business Research, Elsevier, vol. 132(C), pages 604-613.
    5. Shawn N Geniole & Thomas F Denson & Barnaby J Dixson & Justin M Carré & Cheryl M McCormick, 2015. "Evidence from Meta-Analyses of the Facial Width-to-Height Ratio as an Evolved Cue of Threat," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(7), pages 1-18, July.
    6. Barnaby J. Dixson & Jamie C. Tam & Monica Awasthy, 2013. "Do women’s preferences for men’s facial hair change with reproductive status?," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(3), pages 708-716.

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Barnaby J. Dixson & Jamie C. Tam & Monica Awasthy, 2013. "Do women’s preferences for men’s facial hair change with reproductive status?," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 24(3), pages 708-716.

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:23:y:2012:i:3:p:481-490.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.