IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/plo/pone00/0179407.html
   My bibliography  Save this article

Perceived differences in social status between speaker and listener affect the speaker's vocal characteristics

Author

Listed:
  • Juan David Leongómez
  • Viktoria R Mileva
  • Anthony C Little
  • S Craig Roberts

Abstract

Non-verbal behaviours, including voice characteristics during speech, are an important way to communicate social status. Research suggests that individuals can obtain high social status through dominance (using force and intimidation) or through prestige (by being knowledgeable and skilful). However, little is known regarding differences in the vocal behaviour of men and women in response to dominant and prestigious individuals. Here, we tested within-subject differences in vocal parameters of interviewees during simulated job interviews with dominant, prestigious, and neutral employers (targets), while responding to questions which were classified as introductory, personal, and interpersonal. We found that vocal modulations were apparent between responses to the neutral and high-status targets, with participants, especially those who perceived themselves as low in dominance, increasing fundamental frequency (F0) in response to the dominant and prestigious targets relative to the neutral target. Self-perceived prestige, however, was less related to contextual vocal modulations than self-perceived dominance. Finally, we found that differences in the context of the interview questions participants were asked to respond to (introductory, personal, interpersonal), also affected their vocal parameters, being more prominent in responses to personal and interpersonal questions. Overall, our results suggest that people adjust their vocal parameters according to the perceived social status of the listener as well as their own self-perceived social status.

Suggested Citation

  • Juan David Leongómez & Viktoria R Mileva & Anthony C Little & S Craig Roberts, 2017. "Perceived differences in social status between speaker and listener affect the speaker's vocal characteristics," PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 12(6), pages 1-21, June.
  • Handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0179407
    DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179407
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0179407
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0179407&type=printable
    Download Restriction: no

    File URL: https://libkey.io/10.1371/journal.pone.0179407?utm_source=ideas
    LibKey link: if access is restricted and if your library uses this service, LibKey will redirect you to where you can use your library subscription to access this item
    ---><---

    References listed on IDEAS

    as
    1. Carolyn R. Hodges-Simeon & Michael Gurven & David A. Puts & Steven J.C. Gaulin, 2014. "Vocal fundamental and formant frequencies are honest signals of threat potential in peripubertal males," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(4), pages 984-988.
    2. Christopher D. Watkins & Paul J. Fraccaro & Finlay G. Smith & Jovana Vukovic & David R. Feinberg & Lisa M. DeBruine & Benedict C. Jones, 2010. "Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 21(5), pages 943-947.
    Full references (including those not matched with items on IDEAS)

    Most related items

    These are the items that most often cite the same works as this one and are cited by the same works as this one.
    1. Christopher D. Watkins & Benedict C. Jones, 2012. "Priming men with different contest outcomes modulates their dominance perceptions," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 23(3), pages 539-543.
    2. Bittmann, Felix, 2020. "The relationship between height and leadership: Evidence from across Europe," Economics & Human Biology, Elsevier, vol. 36(C).

    More about this item

    Statistics

    Access and download statistics

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:plo:pone00:0179407. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    If CitEc recognized a bibliographic reference but did not link an item in RePEc to it, you can help with this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: plosone (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/ .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.