Author
Listed:
- Amélie Yavchitz
- Isabelle Boutron
- Aida Bafeta
- Ibrahim Marroun
- Pierre Charles
- Jean Mantz
- Philippe Ravaud
Abstract
A study conducted by Amélie Yavchitz and colleagues examines the factors associated with “spin” (specific reporting strategies, intentional or unintentional, that emphasize the beneficial effect of treatments) in press releases of clinical trials. Background: Previous studies indicate that in published reports, trial results can be distorted by the use of “spin” (specific reporting strategies, intentional or unintentional, emphasizing the beneficial effect of the experimental treatment). We aimed to (1) evaluate the presence of “spin” in press releases and associated media coverage; and (2) evaluate whether findings of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) based on press releases and media coverage are misinterpreted. Methods and Findings: We systematically searched for all press releases indexed in the EurekAlert! database between December 2009 and March 2010. Of the 498 press releases retrieved and screened, we included press releases for all two-arm, parallel-group RCTs (n = 70). We obtained a copy of the scientific article to which the press release related and we systematically searched for related news items using Lexis Nexis. Conclusion: “Spin” was identified in about half of press releases and media coverage. In multivariable analysis, the main factor associated with “spin” in press releases was the presence of “spin” in the article abstract conclusion. Background: The mass media play an important role in disseminating the results of medical research. Every day, news items in newspapers and magazines and on the television, radio, and internet provide the general public with information about the latest clinical studies. Such news items are written by journalists and are often based on information in “press releases.” These short communications, which are posted on online databases such as EurekAlert! and sent directly to journalists, are prepared by researchers or more often by the drug companies, funding bodies, or institutions supporting the clinical research and are designed to attract favorable media attention to newly published research results. Press releases provide journalists with the information they need to develop and publish a news story, including a link to the peer-reviewed journal (a scholarly periodical containing articles that have been judged by independent experts) in which the research results appear. Why Was This Study Done?: In an ideal world, journal articles, press releases, and news stories would all accurately reflect the results of health research. Unfortunately, the findings of randomized controlled trials (RCTs—studies that compare the outcomes of patients randomly assigned to receive alternative interventions), which are the best way to evaluate new treatments, are sometimes distorted in peer-reviewed journals by the use of “spin”—reporting that emphasizes the beneficial effects of the experimental (new) treatment. For example, a journal article may interpret nonstatistically significant differences as showing the equivalence of two treatments although such results actually indicate a lack of evidence for the superiority of either treatment. “Spin” can distort the transposition of research into clinical practice and, when reproduced in the mass media, it can give patients unrealistic expectations about new treatments. It is important, therefore, to know where “spin” occurs and to understand the effects of that “spin”. In this study, the researchers evaluate the presence of “spin” in press releases and associated media coverage and analyze whether the interpretation of RCT results based on press releases and associated news items could lead to the misinterpretation of RCT results. What Did the Researchers Do and Find?: The researchers identified 70 press releases indexed in EurekAlert! over a 4-month period that described two-arm, parallel-group RCTs. They used Lexis Nexis, a database of news reports from around the world, to identify associated news items for 41 of these press releases and then analyzed the press releases, news items, and abstracts of the scientific articles related to each press release for “spin”. Finally, they interpreted the results of the RCTs using each source of information independently. Nearly half the press releases and article abstract conclusions contained “spin” and, importantly, “spin” in the press releases was associated with “spin” in the article abstracts. The researchers overestimated the benefits of the experimental treatment from the press release as compared to the full-text peer-reviewed article for 27% of reports. Factors that were associated with this overestimation of treatment benefits included publication in a specialized journal and having “spin” in the press release. Of the news items related to press releases, half contained “spin”, usually of the same type as identified in the press release and article abstract. Finally, the researchers overestimated the benefit of the experimental treatment from the news item as compared to the full-text peer-reviewed article in 24% of cases. What Do These Findings Mean?: These findings show that “spin” in press releases and news reports is related to the presence of “spin” in the abstract of peer-reviewed reports of RCTs and suggest that the interpretation of RCT results based solely on press releases or media coverage could distort the interpretation of research findings in a way that favors experimental treatments. This interpretation shift is probably related to the presence of “spin” in peer-reviewed article abstracts, press releases, and news items and may be partly responsible for a mismatch between the perceived and real beneficial effects of new treatments among the general public. Overall, these findings highlight the important role that journal reviewers and editors play in disseminating research findings. These individuals, the researchers conclude, have a responsibility to ensure that the conclusions reported in the abstracts of peer-reviewed articles are appropriate and do not over-interpret the results of clinical research. Additional Information: Please access these Web sites via the online version of this summary at http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308.
Suggested Citation
Amélie Yavchitz & Isabelle Boutron & Aida Bafeta & Ibrahim Marroun & Pierre Charles & Jean Mantz & Philippe Ravaud, 2012.
"Misrepresentation of Randomized Controlled Trials in Press Releases and News Coverage: A Cohort Study,"
PLOS Medicine, Public Library of Science, vol. 9(9), pages 1-11, September.
Handle:
RePEc:plo:pmed00:1001308
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001308
Download full text from publisher
Citations
Citations are extracted by the
CitEc Project, subscribe to its
RSS feed for this item.
Cited by:
- João A G Moreira & Xiao Han T Zeng & Luís A Nunes Amaral, 2015.
"The Distribution of the Asymptotic Number of Citations to Sets of Publications by a Researcher or from an Academic Department Are Consistent with a Discrete Lognormal Model,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(11), pages 1-17, November.
- Markus Lehmkuhl & Nikolai Promies, 2020.
"Frequency distribution of journalistic attention for scientific studies and scientific sources: An input–output analysis,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 15(11), pages 1-20, November.
- Michael T M Wang & Mark J Bolland & Greg Gamble & Andrew Grey, 2015.
"Media Coverage, Journal Press Releases and Editorials Associated with Randomized and Observational Studies in High-Impact Medical Journals: A Cohort Study,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(12), pages 1-6, December.
- Arnaud Vaganay, 2016.
"Outcome Reporting Bias in Government-Sponsored Policy Evaluations: A Qualitative Content Analysis of 13 Studies,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 11(9), pages 1-21, September.
- Paula Silva & María P. Portillo & Alfredo Fernández-Quintela, 2022.
"Resveratrol and Wine: An Overview of Thirty Years in the Digital News,"
IJERPH, MDPI, vol. 19(23), pages 1-14, November.
- , Aisdl, 2020.
"Let’s Do Better: Public Representations of COVID-19 Science,"
OSF Preprints
3cpvs, Center for Open Science.
- Robert G. Alexander & Stephen L. Macknik & Susana Martinez-Conde, 2022.
"What the Neuroscience and Psychology of Magic Reveal about Misinformation,"
Publications, MDPI, vol. 10(4), pages 1-19, September.
- Michael T M Wang & Greg Gamble & Mark J Bolland & Andrew Grey, 2014.
"Press Releases Issued by Supplements Industry Organisations and Non-Industry Organisations in Response to Publication of Clinical Research Findings: A Case-Control Study,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 9(7), pages 1-14, July.
- Andrew W Brown & David B Allison, 2014.
"Using Crowdsourcing to Evaluate Published Scientific Literature: Methods and Example,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 9(7), pages 1-9, July.
- Estelle Dumas-Mallet & Andy Smith & Thomas Boraud & François Gonon, 2017.
"Poor replication validity of biomedical association studies reported by newspapers,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 12(2), pages 1-15, February.
- Cohen, Scott & Stienmetz, Jason & Hanna, Paul & Humbracht, Michael & Hopkins, Debbie, 2020.
"Shadowcasting tourism knowledge through media: Self-driving sex cars?,"
Annals of Tourism Research, Elsevier, vol. 85(C).
- Romana Haneef & Clement Lazarus & Philippe Ravaud & Amélie Yavchitz & Isabelle Boutron, 2015.
"Interpretation of Results of Studies Evaluating an Intervention Highlighted in Google Health News: A Cross-Sectional Study of News,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(10), pages 1-15, October.
- Joseph W Taylor & Marie Long & Elizabeth Ashley & Alex Denning & Beatrice Gout & Kayleigh Hansen & Thomas Huws & Leifa Jennings & Sinead Quinn & Patrick Sarkies & Alex Wojtowicz & Philip M Newton, 2015.
"When Medical News Comes from Press Releases—A Case Study of Pancreatic Cancer and Processed Meat,"
PLOS ONE, Public Library of Science, vol. 10(6), pages 1-13, June.
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:plo:pmed00:1001308. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.
We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .
If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: plosmedicine (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://journals.plos.org/plosmedicine/ .
Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through
the various RePEc services.