IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/geronb/v75y2020i9pe242-e248..html
   My bibliography  Save this article

“We” Before “Me”: Differences in Usage of Collectivistic and Individualistic Language Influence Judgments of Electability and Performance

Author

Listed:
  • Jennifer R Turner
  • Jennifer Tehan Stanley
  • Lynn Martire

Abstract

ObjectivesOlder adults are often judged to be warm, but not competent, which contradicts their representation in positions of authority. This study sought to extend evidence of age differences in more individualistic (e.g., “I”) and collectivistic (e.g., “we”) language and explore their impact on judgments of performance and electability.MethodSpeeches from young and older adults who campaigned for a fictitious position were analyzed using Linguistic and Inquiry Word Count Software. Words fitting specified categories (e.g., pronouns, affect) were compared to outcome judgments obtained from trained coders on the dimensions of performance and electability.ResultsOlder adults used significantly more “we”-language. Young adults used more “I”-language, and more positive affect, achievement, and power language. Language choices and coder judgments were associated such that the more “I”-language that was used during the speech, the less electable the candidate was judged. This relationship was not found for “we”-language.DiscussionThis study found no evidence for collectivistic language enhancing ratings of electability or performance; however, an age-invariable, negative relationship was obtained between increased individualistic language and reduced coder judgments of electability. This suggests that speakers should minimize “I”-statements to promote electability, a characteristic that is reflected more in older adults’ speeches than young.

Suggested Citation

  • Jennifer R Turner & Jennifer Tehan Stanley & Lynn Martire, 2020. "“We” Before “Me”: Differences in Usage of Collectivistic and Individualistic Language Influence Judgments of Electability and Performance," The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, The Gerontological Society of America, vol. 75(9), pages 242-248.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:geronb:v:75:y:2020:i:9:p:e242-e248.
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/geronb/gbz030
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:geronb:v:75:y:2020:i:9:p:e242-e248.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.