IDEAS home Printed from https://ideas.repec.org/a/oup/beheco/v30y2019i5p1388-1397..html
   My bibliography  Save this article

The evolution of two types of play

Author

Listed:
  • Paul E Smaldino
  • Elisabetta Palagi
  • Gordon M Burghardt
  • Sergio M Pellis
  • John Quinn

Abstract

Play is an important and understudied class of phenomena that likely serves a critical role in the ontogeny and maintenance of fitness-enhancing behaviors. Many species exhibit little or no play. Among those animals that do play, some exhibit only very simple forms, while others engage in complex play both solitarily and socially. Likewise, some animals play only as juveniles, while others continue to play as adults. We propose a general framework to explain interspecies variation in the evolution and emergence of simple vs. complex forms of play, supported by both a review of the empirical evidence and a novel mathematical model. The emergence of play requires that initial investment returns benefits that sufficiently compensate the opportunity costs associated with simple play. The subsequent evolution of complex play depends upon the interplay of several life-history factors related to the benefits, costs, and time course of play investment. We conclude with implications for understanding the evolution of play across the animal kingdom. Animals vary greatly in how much they play. Why? Some species do not appear to exhibit play at all. Some exhibit only simple, often solitary, play behavior when juvenile, while others engage in complex, often social, play behavior that extends into adulthood. We discuss the evolutionary benefits and costs of play, review interspecies variation, and discuss how that variation relates to the evolutionary tradeoffs during life history. We structure our discussion around a simple mathematical model.

Suggested Citation

  • Paul E Smaldino & Elisabetta Palagi & Gordon M Burghardt & Sergio M Pellis & John Quinn, 2019. "The evolution of two types of play," Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 30(5), pages 1388-1397.
  • Handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:30:y:2019:i:5:p:1388-1397.
    as

    Download full text from publisher

    File URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arz090
    Download Restriction: Access to full text is restricted to subscribers.
    ---><---

    As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.

    Corrections

    All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:30:y:2019:i:5:p:1388-1397.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.

    If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.

    We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .

    If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.

    For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .

    Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through the various RePEc services.

    IDEAS is a RePEc service. RePEc uses bibliographic data supplied by the respective publishers.