Author
Listed:
- Ivonne Meuche
- Alexander Keller
- Hanyrol H. Ahmad Sah
- Norhayati Ahmad
- T. Ulmar Grafe
Abstract
Eavesdropping predators and parasites that exploit the mating signals of their prey often prefer to approach certain signal variants. However, victim finding depends on many different factors. Therefore, preferences of eavesdroppers may not always directly reflect the predation or parasitism risk of the signaler. Frog-biting midges (Corethrella spp.) and their anuran hosts offer an excellent model system to investigate this correlation. Female Corethrella are vectors of trypanosomes in frogs and use the mating calls of their anuran hosts to localize them. We tested whether the acoustic preferences of Corethrella reflects the parasitism risk of male frogs by Corethrella and trypanosomes in the field. We conducted acoustic attraction and preference tests with Corethrella in the field and used the results of the preference tests to estimate a linear model (LM) and predict the parasitism risk of sympatric frog species by Corethrella. Our preference tests in the field showed that frog-biting midges preferred higher call rates, longer calls, a frequency around 2kHz, and sounds that were broadcast near the ground. Anuran species that were predicted by our LM to produce the most attractive signal showed the highest rates of parasitism by frog-biting midges and trypanosomes. Thus, acoustic preferences of eavesdropping Corethrella can be used to predict not only the parasitism rate of their host species by Corethrella but also the risk of subsequent trypanosome transmission.
Suggested Citation
Ivonne Meuche & Alexander Keller & Hanyrol H. Ahmad Sah & Norhayati Ahmad & T. Ulmar Grafe, 2016.
"Silent listeners: can preferences of eavesdropping midges predict their hosts’ parasitism risk?,"
Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 27(4), pages 995-1003.
Handle:
RePEc:oup:beheco:v:27:y:2016:i:4:p:995-1003.
Download full text from publisher
As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:27:y:2016:i:4:p:995-1003.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.
We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .
If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .
Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through
the various RePEc services.