Author
Listed:
- Arne Janssen
- Juliana Oliveira Fonseca
- Felipe Colares
- Lidiane Silva
- Aline R.P. Pedrosa
- Eraldo R. Lima
- Michiel van Wijk
- Angelo Pallini
- Cleber M. Oliveira
- Maurice W. Sabelis
- Izabela Lesna
Abstract
Many animals use volatile chemicals to detect and locate their food, but they frequently have to cope with a large variation in volatile blends associated with food. It has often been suggested that they do this by learning the association between odors and the presence of food. Indeed, associative learning was demonstrated for a range of animals under laboratory conditions and individuals usually take seconds to hours to modify their behavior. However, it is poorly understood at which timescale field populations and communities of animal species respond. We studied this by exposing egg batches of arthropod herbivores, half of which were combined with a volatile, to a community of natural enemies in the field. We used mint oil as a volatile blend, which we showed not to be attractive in the laboratory for 2 predators that commonly occur in the field. The predation of egg batches with volatiles in the field was initially slightly lower than that of batches without volatiles, indicating that the odor was not innately attractive to the natural enemies. However, relative to the controls, the predation of eggs associated with volatiles increased significantly over a period of 4–5 days. This suggests that individuals in the community of natural enemies associated the presence of eggs with the volatiles. We observed 3 different types of predation, indicating that various groups of predator species were responsible for predation in the field. Our results show that communities of animals rapidly associate volatiles with food in the field.
Suggested Citation
Arne Janssen & Juliana Oliveira Fonseca & Felipe Colares & Lidiane Silva & Aline R.P. Pedrosa & Eraldo R. Lima & Michiel van Wijk & Angelo Pallini & Cleber M. Oliveira & Maurice W. Sabelis & Izabela L, 2014.
"Time scales of associating food and odor by predator communities in the field,"
Behavioral Ecology, International Society for Behavioral Ecology, vol. 25(5), pages 1123-1130.
Handle:
RePEc:oup:beheco:v:25:y:2014:i:5:p:1123-1130.
Download full text from publisher
As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:oup:beheco:v:25:y:2014:i:5:p:1123-1130.. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.
We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .
If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Oxford University Press (email available below). General contact details of provider: https://academic.oup.com/beheco .
Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through
the various RePEc services.