Author
Listed:
- Martin Giurfa
(Neurobiologie, Institut für Biologie, Freie Universität Berlin
Ethologie et Cognition Animale, Universite Paul Sabatier)
- Shaowu Zhang
(Centre for Visual Science, Research School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University, PO Box 475)
- Arnim Jenett
(Neurobiologie, Institut für Biologie, Freie Universität Berlin)
- Randolf Menzel
(Neurobiologie, Institut für Biologie, Freie Universität Berlin)
- Mandyam V. Srinivasan
(Centre for Visual Science, Research School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University, PO Box 475)
Abstract
Insects process and learn information flexibly to adapt to their environment. The honeybee Apis mellifera constitutes a traditional model for studying learning and memory at behavioural, cellular and molecular levels1. Earlier studies focused on elementary associative and non-associative forms of learning determined by either olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension reflex1 or the learning of visual stimuli2 in an operant context. However, research has indicated that bees are capable of cognitive performances that were thought to occur only in some vertebrate species. For example, honeybees can interpolate visual information3, exhibit associative recall4,5, categorize visual information6,7,8 and learn contextual information9. Here we show that honeybees can form ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ concepts. They learn to solve ‘delayed matching-to-sample’ tasks, in which they are required to respond to a matching stimulus, and ‘delayed non-matching-to-sample’ tasks, in which they are required to respond to a different stimulus; they can also transfer the learned rules to new stimuli of the same or a different sensory modality. Thus, not only can bees learn specific objects and their physical parameters, but they can also master abstract inter-relationships, such as sameness and difference.
Suggested Citation
Martin Giurfa & Shaowu Zhang & Arnim Jenett & Randolf Menzel & Mandyam V. Srinivasan, 2001.
"The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect,"
Nature, Nature, vol. 410(6831), pages 930-933, April.
Handle:
RePEc:nat:nature:v:410:y:2001:i:6831:d:10.1038_35073582
DOI: 10.1038/35073582
Download full text from publisher
As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:410:y:2001:i:6831:d:10.1038_35073582. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.
We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .
If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .
Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through
the various RePEc services.