Author
Listed:
- Miguel Castelo-Branco
(Max-Planck-Institut für Hirnforschung)
- Rainer Goebel
(Max-Planck-Institut für Hirnforschung)
- Sergio Neuenschwander
(Max-Planck-Institut für Hirnforschung)
- Wolf Singer
(Max-Planck-Institut für Hirnforschung)
Abstract
To analyse an image, the visual system must decompose the scene into its relevant parts. Identifying distinct surfaces is a basic operation in such analysis, and is believed to precede object recognition1,2. Two superimposed gratings moving in different directions (plaid stimuli) may be perceived either as two surfaces, one being transparent and sliding on top of the other (component motion) or as a single pattern whose direction of motion is intermediate to the component vectors (pattern motion)3,4,5,6. The degree of transparency, and hence the perception, can be manipulated by changing only the luminance of the grating intersections7,8,9,10,11,12. Here we show that neurons in two visual cortical areas—A18 and PMLS—synchronize their discharges when responding to contours of the same surface but not when responding to contours belonging to different surfaces. The amplitudes of responses correspond to previously described rate predictions3,13,14,15,16 for component and pattern motion, but, in contrast to synchrony, failed to reflect the transition from component to pattern motion induced by manipulating the degree of transparency. Thus, dynamic changes in synchronization could encode, in a context-dependent way, relations among simultaneous responses to spatially superimposed contours and thereby bias their association with distinct surfaces.
Suggested Citation
Miguel Castelo-Branco & Rainer Goebel & Sergio Neuenschwander & Wolf Singer, 2000.
"Neural synchrony correlates with surface segregation rules,"
Nature, Nature, vol. 405(6787), pages 685-689, June.
Handle:
RePEc:nat:nature:v:405:y:2000:i:6787:d:10.1038_35015079
DOI: 10.1038/35015079
Download full text from publisher
As the access to this document is restricted, you may want to search for a different version of it.
Corrections
All material on this site has been provided by the respective publishers and authors. You can help correct errors and omissions. When requesting a correction, please mention this item's handle: RePEc:nat:nature:v:405:y:2000:i:6787:d:10.1038_35015079. See general information about how to correct material in RePEc.
If you have authored this item and are not yet registered with RePEc, we encourage you to do it here. This allows to link your profile to this item. It also allows you to accept potential citations to this item that we are uncertain about.
We have no bibliographic references for this item. You can help adding them by using this form .
If you know of missing items citing this one, you can help us creating those links by adding the relevant references in the same way as above, for each refering item. If you are a registered author of this item, you may also want to check the "citations" tab in your RePEc Author Service profile, as there may be some citations waiting for confirmation.
For technical questions regarding this item, or to correct its authors, title, abstract, bibliographic or download information, contact: Sonal Shukla or Springer Nature Abstracting and Indexing (email available below). General contact details of provider: http://www.nature.com .
Please note that corrections may take a couple of weeks to filter through
the various RePEc services.